Neuroticism, extraversion, emotion regulation, negative affect and positive affect: The mediating roles of reappraisal and suppression

Main Article Content

Li Wang
Zhanbiao Shi
Huanhuan Li
Cite this article:  Wang, L., Shi, Z., & Li, H. (2009). Neuroticism, extraversion, emotion regulation, negative affect and positive affect: The mediating roles of reappraisal and suppression. Social Behavior and Personality: An international journal, 37(2), 193-194.


Abstract
Full Text
References
Tables and Figures
Acknowledgments
Author Contact

The associations between extraversion and positive affect and between neuroticism and negative affect are well documented (e.g., Diener, Oishi, & Lucas, 2003). However, it is still unclear why the extraversion-positive affect and neuroticism-negative affect relationships are so close. Based on the findings from emotion regulation research (e.g., John & Gross, 2004), the mediating roles of two emotion regulation strategies - reappraisal and suppression - between the two personality dimensions and positive and negative affect were examined in this study.

A thousand undergraduate students were recruited from four universities in China; 430 men, ages ranging from 16.42 to 24 years (M = 20.59, SD = 1.24). Participants completed a package of questionnaires including: the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Short Scale (EPQ-RS; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1991), Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003) and general dimension scales of Positive Affect and Negative Affect -Expanded Form (PANAS-X; Watson & Clark, 1994). The structural equation modeling technique was employed to test and modify the hypothetical mediation model, and bootstrap methods were used to test the significance levels of indirect effects. The results indicated that neuroticism and extraversion contributed to negative affect and positive affect indirectly through reappraisal beside the direct contribution respectively, and suppression had no mediating roles between neuroticism and negative affect or between extraversion and positive affect (see Figure 1). Furthermore, neuroticism and extraversion explained 22% of the variance in reappraisal, and neuroticism and reappraisal, and extraversion and reappraisal explained 42% and 28% of the variance in negative affect and positive affect, respectively.

Table/Figure

Figure 1. The final mediation model.
Note: ** p < .01

These results suggest that individuals with different personality dimensions (i.e., extraversion and neuroticism) use emotion regulation strategies differently (i.e., reappraisal), which in turn influences the affect they experience. These results provide empirical evidence to support the proposition that the link between personality and affect is not simply a direct relationship but, rather both that extraversion contributes to positive affect and neuroticism contributes to negative affect through emotion regulation, one of important psychological processes. Though there is still a long way to fully clarify the mediating roles of emotion regulation, the findings of this pilot study provide a new insight for us to understand the extraversion-positive affect and neuroticism-negative affect
relationships.

References

Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403-425.

Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1991). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Scales. London: Hodder & Stoughton Publishers.

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348-362.

John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301-1334.

Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa.

Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403-425.

Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1991). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Scales. London: Hodder & Stoughton Publishers.

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348-362.

John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301-1334.

Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded form. Iowa City: University of Iowa.

Table/Figure

Figure 1. The final mediation model.
Note: ** p < .01


Appreciation is due to anonymous reviewers.

Zhanbiao Shi, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 4A Datun Road, Beijing 100101, People’s Republic of China. Phone: +86-10-6485-5883; Fax: +86-10-6487-2070; Email: [email protected]

Article Details

© 2009 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved.